This website uses only technical or equivalent cookies.
For more information click here.

Abstract

Anhedonia, defined as the loss of pleasure or a lack of reactivity to pleasurable stimuli, is associated with depression, schizophrenia and also anorexia nervosa (AN). The inability to experience pleasure has its biological underpinnings in the mesolimbic dopamine system and is strongly implicated in the risk for a variety of addictive behaviors. In this article we describe the main questionnaires for the assessment of anhedonia and review the literature evidence regarding anhedonic symptomatology in patients diagnosed with AN. This narrative review, in agreement with previous literature, sheds light on the importance of considering anhedonia as a phenotypic biomarker and suggests implementing the development of targeted treatments.

Introduction

Anhedonia, defined as the inability or the reduced ability to experience pleasure 1, was first described in the previous century as a symptom of depression 2,3. Reward induces subjective feelings of pleasure or positive emotional states promoting approach to pleasurable stimuli, consummatory behaviors and maintenance of rewarding behaviors. Anhedonia can result from impairments in different steps of reward processing, including reward desire, anticipation of pleasure, effort to achieve pleasure, consuming pleasure, learning from rewarding stimuli. The experience of pleasure can be induced by instinctual stimuli (e.g., food, sex) and non-instinctual stimuli (e.g., photography, reading); the former represent “primary” rewards (inherent rewards), while the latter are secondary rewards (without inherent reward in itself and for which reward value must be learned).

Anhedonia has been found to be a core symptom of several neuropsychiatric disorders, including major depressive disorder 4, schizophrenia 5, bipolar disorder, obsessive compulsive disorder 6, Parkinson’s disease 7, eating disorders (EDs) and maladaptive behaviors and 8,9,2. Anhedonia is associated also with multiple negative health behaviors, such as smoking, illicit drug use, and poor physical activity, even in the absence of psychiatric disorders 10,11. In line with the Research Domain Criteria approach 12, state anhedonia can be measured and studied as a dimensional psychopathological trait. Moreover, anhedonia can be considered a stable personality trait, able to predict future psychopathology 13-15.

Recently, the role of anhedonia in EDs has been emphasized because of the more and more frequent evidence of alterations in reward processing in these disorders 16.

It is widely acknowledged that in AN anhedonia might play an important role in both the etiopathogenesis and maintenance of eating pathology. In support of this, it has been found that restrictive eating is maintained by deficits in positive emotions and that individuals with AN experience pleasure from restrictive eating behaviors but not from typically rewarding stimuli 17.

In this narrative review we briefly illustrate the psychometric instruments allowing the measurement of individual anhedonia levels and then review literature studies that assessed anhedonia in individuals with AN pointing to its clinical significance in both the presentation and the management of the EDs.

Assessment of anhedonia

The main methods for the assessment of anhedonia can be distinguished into “first generation” questionnaires and “second generation” anhedonia scales, which differ because the latter, developed more recently, are aimed at exploring all the different facets of reward functions 18.

The first-generation questionnaires mostly used in clinical research are the Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale (SHAPS) 19, the Fawcett-Clark Pleasure Capacity Scale (FCPS) 20, the Revised Chapman Physical Anhedonia Scale (CPAS), and the Chapman Social Anhedonia Scale (CSAS) 21. Different aspects of anhedonia are assessed by these four scales. The CPAS and CSAS measure various aspects of anhedonia (motivation, effort, and consummatory pleasure) in addition to personality traits, while both the FCPS and SHAPS focus exclusively on consummatory pleasure. All these scales incorporate questions relating to both primary and secondary reward. The SHAPS, in particular, devised items based on the domains of pastimes, social interaction, food/drink, achievement and sensory experience. This is in contrast to the CPAS and CSAS, which separate physical and social anhedonia. The revised version of the CSAS, namely the Revised Social Anhedonia Scale (RSAS) 22 ranks among the most widely used questionnaires. The RSAS is a 40-item true-false self-report questionnaire that assesses the ability to experience pleasure in social interactions.

Second-generation scales include: 1) the Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scale (TEPS), with 18 items and two subscales designed to distinguish between anticipatory and consummatory pleasure 23; 2) the Motivation and Pleasure Scale-Self Report (MAP-SR) to measure negative symptoms in schizophrenia 24; 3) the Specific Loss of Interest Scale (SLIPS) 25; 4) the Anticipatory and Consummatory Interpersonal Pleasure Scale (ACIPS) 26,27, a 17- item self-report scale that focuses on measuring anhedonia related to social interactions; 5) the Dimensional Anhedonia Rating Scale (DARS), a 17-item self-report questionnaire that was designed to assess anhedonia in Major Depressive Disorder 28. The 18 TEPS items reflect only physical anhedonia, as the authors believed that this way the results would be more homogeneous and interpretable. The scale was validated using a population of students and has been employed, as far as we know, only in psychiatric populations with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder 29,30. The MAP-SR contains 15 questions across four domains: social pleasure, recreational or work pleasure, feelings and motivations about close, caring relationships, motivation and effort to engage in activities. The questions are answered on a 4-point Likert scale, where a high score is indicative of low anhedonia. The SLIPS was primarily designed to measure levels of interest over time (a 2-week period). Each of the 23-items has a 3-point scale, with higher scores denoting increasing levels of anhedonia. The SLIPS demonstrated high reliability as well as convergent validity with the SHAPS, moderate correlation with the TEPS anticipatory scale and small correlation with the TEPS consummatory scale. As for the ACIPS, the focus on social anhedonia is derived from research reporting high levels of social anhedonia as risk factor for the development of schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. Although the revised CSAS measures the same construct of the ACIPS, it does not distinguish between anticipation and consummatory pleasure, and it is unclear whether the CSAS would be useful in youth or adolescent groups due to the age bias of the items. The ACIPS was specifically designed to address these limitations as well as to provide a shorter scale, which would be more useful in clinical settings.

In addition to the above-mentioned instruments, the Behavioral Inhibition System and the Behavioral Activation System (BIS/BAS) scale was developed to measure personality traits of behavioral approach and inhibition based on Gray’s theory of behaviour, the concept being that there are two distinct neurological systems for aversive motivation and appetitive motivation 31.The BIS system is activated in response to non-rewarding, novel stimuli that can lead to negative outcomes and inhibits behavior towards these stimuli. The BAS system, in contrast, is activated by rewarding stimuli. The BIS/BAS is a 24-item self-report scale with 4 factors: one for the BIS and 3 subscales for the BAS (drive, reward response and fun seeking).

Anhedonia in anorexia nervosa

AN is an ED characterized by abnormally low body weight associated with intense fear of gaining weight and distorted cognitions regarding body weight and shape 32. Although its etiology is still unknown, several pathogenetic models involving brain systems associated with cognitive self-regulation, motivation/reward processing, and appetite regulation have been proposed 33-43. A key aspect of those models is a “top-down” versus “bottom-up” element, in which “top-down” modulatory mechanisms reflect the prefrontal cognitive control, while “bottom-up” limbic and interoceptive processes reflect the integration of inputs from systems that regulate factors such as homeostatic need, reward responses, and motivational drive. It has been proposed that, in AN, hyperactivation of “top-down” cognitive control networks exerts control over “bottom-up” appetitive responses, and that this, symptomatically, emerges as a pathological desire to be thin, as elevated anticipatory anxiety when exposed to food, and as behavioral rigidity including excessive control over food intake 38,43-46.

The influence of motivation on eating behaviors suggested that dysfunctional neural circuits involved in reward and punishment processing contribute to the development and maintenance of AN and other EDs 47-50. Abnormalities in reward and punishment processing have been framed neurobiologically in the mesolimbic dopamine (DA) reward system 49-52. DA has certainly been the most studied candidate as associated with reward in AN. Increased binding of D2 and D3 receptors in the striatum of women with AN compared to healthy controls has been reported by a positron emission tomography study 53. In addition, central dopaminergic activity 54, peripherally measured by means of spontaneous eye blink rate, has been shown to be abnormally elevated in AN patients compared to healthy controls 55. Most studies suggest that DA is primarily involved in the “desirable” component of reward, that is, in the attribution of motivational salience to reward-related stimuli. In AN a sensitization of the DA reward system may occur, thus enabling the amplification of the incentive value of stimuli/behaviors compatible with disease goals (e.g., thinness and exercise). Furthermore, similarly to drug addiction, it has been proposed that in AN dietary restriction and elevated drive to exercise are rewarding behaviors associated with addiction, as they persist despite their negative physical, emotional, and social consequences 32,56-59. Therefore, the existence of DA-mediated neural mechanisms underlying the reinforcing values of aberrant eating behaviors similarly to the aberrant behaviors of individuals with drug abuse has been hypothesized 60.

The role of a dysfunctional DA system in AN is further corroborated by studies in which individuals with AN appeared ascetic and anhedonic 61, and anhedonia results from impairments in various aspects of reward processing and is associated with poorer recovery and/or a more severe clinical presentation of AN. Moreover, anhedonia should not be seen only as an inducible state, dependent upon malnutrition and/or concomitant depressive symptomatology, but also as an innate character trait. Indeed, it seems that anhedonia reflects a weakness of physiological positive reinforcers and that within the general population, reward capacity exists as a normally distributed dimension 8.

In AN, primary rewards are frequently described as unpleasant and are avoided by AN patients. These features have been seen as an anhedonia-like phenotype 62,63, suggesting that patients have an impaired ability to experience pleasure or reward. The anhedonic construct includes physical anhedonia, an inability to experience physical pleasure, and social anhedonia, a decreased ability to take pleasure in social activities 21. Physical anhedonia can be measured by items such as “I would like to have a cup of tea or coffee or my favorite beverage” or “I would be able to enjoy a beautiful landscape or view19, while social anhedonia can be measured by items such as “Just being with friends makes me feel very good” and “I have always enjoyed looking at photographs of friends21. Both components of anhedonia have been explored in people with AN. Moreover, according to the above-mentioned Gray’s theory of behaviour 31 some studies have assessed also aversive and appetitive motivation of people with AN by means of the BIS/BAS scale (Tab. I).

Social anhedonia in anorexia nervosa

Eiber et al. 64 tested the hedonic response and the fear of weight gain after ingestion of a sucrose solution in a sample of patients with AN-R, AN-BP and BN measuring also the social and physical anhedonia. They found that social anhedonia scores did not significantly differ among the diagnostic groups but in all the ED group they significantly correlated positively with low mood (measured by Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale), drive for thinness and fear of swallowing sucrose solution and negatively with maximal hedonic response to sucrose solution ingestion. The authors suggested that social factors may be implicated in motivating the patients’ fear of gaining weight and in triggering a decrease of pleasantness rating.

Some studies evaluated social anhedonia in AN individuals compared with healthy controls. Tchanturia et al. 65 and Harrison et al. 66 assessed self-reported social anhedonia through the RSAS in individuals with AN, recovered AN, BN, and healthy controls, showing that patients with AN and BN had higher levels of social anhedonia than healthy controls; recovered AN did not report significant differences with healthy controls. However, social anhedonia was associated with longer illness duration and, together with severity of body weight loss, significantly predicted work/social difficulties 66. According to previous findings 64, patients with AN did not show higher levels of social anhedonia than individuals with BN.

Lang et al. 67, exploring the correlation between facial expressions and some clinical variables including social anhedonia, obtained results similar to previous studies with greater social anhedonia in AN individuals compared to healthy controls.

Other studies have assessed social anhedonia using RSAS in AN individuals before and after Cognitive Remediation and Emotion Skill Training (CREST) 68-71. While Tchanturia et al. 68,69 and Harrison et al. 71 reported significant decrease in RSAS scores following CREST, Adamson et al. 70 did not find any significant change in social anhedonia in AN people undergoing CREST in both the individual and group formats.

Physical anhedonia in anorexia nervosa

Davis and Scott-Robertson 72 compared anhedonia through the CPAS in patients with AN-R and in a group of bodybuilders, revealing that both groups had very high levels of physical anhedonia. Similar results were obtained by Eiber et al. 64, who showed that physical anhedonia levels did not significantly differ among subjects with AN-R, AN-BP or BN, although scores on CPAS were approximately 20 points higher in EDs patients than HC assessed in a previous study 73. In a subsequent study Davis and Woodside 61 compared patients with AN, BN and ED not otherwise specified (EDNOS), dichotomized according to their exercise status in exercising patients (who exercised more than 1 hour per day, for at least 6 days per week, for a period of 1 month or more) and non-exercising patients, to test the idea that anhedonia might be related to high levels of physical activity, the latter as a compensatory mechanism. Results demonstrated that, among patients without excessive exercise, levels of physical anhedonia were higher in AN patients than individuals with BN or EDNOS, whereas among participants with excessive exercise, there were no significant differences in anhedonia scores among all the diagnostic groups.

Jiang et al. 74 in a first study compared patients with AN-R to HC and in a subsequent study 75 included also patients with BN. Although in both studies, AN-R patients had a higher score on CPAS than healthy controls, the difference failed to reach significance. Patients with BN, instead presented higher levels of physical anhedonia than healthy controls.

Some evidence, regarding changes in physical anhedonia after treatments, was provided by a study 76 that assessed anhedonia and depressive symptoms, by means of the SHAPS-D and BDI, respectively, before and after weight restoration in AN individuals. Results indicated there was a significant decrease in depression after weight recovery, but no significant change in anhedonia.

In this study, a significant positive correlation also emerged between ED psychopathology as measured by the Eating Disorder Inventory II and anhedonia as measured by the SHAPS-D in individuals with acute AN.

Dolan et al. 77 measured anhedonia in AN individuals and other EDs on admission and at discharge from a partial hospitalization program. Results indicated that the AN-BP group showed the highest levels of anhedonia, and the Avoidant Restrictive Food Intake Disorder group reported the lowest levels of anhedonia and had a significantly lower anhedonia than the other diagnostic groups. Furthermore, differently from Boehm et al’s study 76, anhedonia scores significantly decreased from admission to discharge. Therefore, while results of Boehm et al. 76 suggest that in AN anhedonia might be not related to malnutrition, the findings of Dolan et al. 77 support the opposite. Thus, the stait or trait nature of anhedonia in AN remains to be elucidated.

Dolan et al. 78 used the TEPS, which exclusively assesses physical anhedonia and distinguishes between anticipatory and consummatory pleasure, to compare patients with AN, BN, other EDs and healthy controls. No significant differences in TEPS consummatory scores emerged between individuals with AN, BN, Other Specified Feeding or Eating Disorder (OSFED) and healthy controls, suggesting that individuals with EDs experience similar levels of consummatory pleasure of individuals with no history of disease. In contrast, anticipatory pleasure was found to be significantly higher in healthy controls than in patients with AN and OSFED. Finally, comparison between diagnostic groups revealed no significant differences among patients with AN, BN and OSFED.

The behavioral inhibition - Behavioral activation system in anorexia nervosa

Four studies have evaluated the Behavioral- Inhibition Behavioral Activation System in patients with AN and with other EDs.

All the studies 79-82 consistently demonstrated that AN-R patients have significantly lower scores on the BAS- Fun Seeking than people with AN-BP, BN and healthy controls 79,82.

Moreover, Claes et al. 79 showed that individuals with AN-R exhibited higher scores on the BIS scale than those with AN-BP or BN or HC. Similarly, Monteleone et al al. 82 reported higher scores on punishment sensitivity (BIS score) of AN and BN patients compared to HC.

In summary, these findings seem to suggest that patients with AN-R are more likely to engage behaviors that can lead to negative outcome, whereas they are less prone to look for rewarding stimuli.

Conclusions

Our review of the literature supports anhedonia as an explanatory model of the reward-related abnormalities reported in AN. Indeed, people with AN have been found to report, although not unanimously, greater levels of both social and physical anhedonia than healthy controls.

In addition, it was found that greater anhedonia was correlated with a number of variables indicating greater ED severity, including general measures of ED psychopathology 76, longer disease duration66, and excessive physical exercise 61.

Studies comparing anhedonia and depressive symptomatology in the AN before and after weight recovery showed improvement in depressive symptoms but were not consistent in anhedonia outcome76. Indeed, some studies reported amelioration of anhedonia after treatment, whereas some others did not. If the last case will be confirmed by future larger studies, anhedonia could be framed as a trait symptom and not a state symptom and would contribute to make anhedonia a separate treatment target and not a symptom included in the treatment of depressive symptomatology.

Anyway, despite current discrepancies in results, there is agreement on implementing targeted treatment for anhedonia. Indeed, delineating an anhedonic phenotype, and thus targeted treatment, would improve the prognosis of AN, considering that patients with high levels of anhedonia are also those with greater work/social difficulties 66.

Finally, future research should focus on how anhedonia correlates with components of reward processing in EDs, improving theoretical models and consequently therapeutic strategies.

Conflict of interest statement

The Authors declare no conflict of interest.

Funding

None.

Authors’ contributions

RC and GC contributed in conception, proposal, data collection and analysis, and manuscript preparation

Ethical consideration

Not applicable.

Figures and tables

Author year Assesment scale Subjects Main results
Davis and Scott-Robertson 72 2000 CPAS 46 subjects with AN-R22 bodybuilders Both the bodybuilders and the AN patients had very high anhedonia scores, although these did not significantly differ.
Eiber et al. 64 2002 CPAS, CSAS 20 subjects with AN-R20 subjects with AN-BP20 subjects with BN Scores on the Social and Physical Anhedonia Scales did not differ significantly among diagnostic groups.Although not significant, scores on the Physical Anhedonia Scale were approximately 20 points higher in ED patients than in control subjects (assessed in a previous study).
Davis and Woodside 61 2002 CPAS 78 subjects with AN-R76 subjects with BN32 subjects with ED not otherwise specified (EDNOS) who no longer met weight criteria for AN (but who had had a previous diagnosis of AN-R) and had no binge-purge symptomatologyPatients were dichotomized according to their exercise status Patients with AN-R showed higher levels of anhedonia compared to those with BN.BN patients who did not exercise excessively had significantly lower anhedonia scores than the other groups, which did not differ from each other.The non-exercising EDNOS group had significantly lower anhedonia scores than the AN non-exercisers.
Claes et al. 79 2002 BIS/BAS 20 subjects with AN-R14 subjects with AN-BP22 subjects with BN83 healthy controls AN-BP, BN and HC individuals scored higher on the BIS-BAS-Fun Seeking than AN-R individuals.AN-R individuals were more behaviorally inhibited (BIS-BAS-Behavioral inhibition) than AN-BP, BN and HC individuals.
Beck et al. 80 2009 BIS-BAS 32 subjects with AN-R17 subjects with AN-BP35 subjects with BN15 subjects with EDNOS The different types of ED patients did not significantly differ from each other with respect to BIS reactivity.AN-R individuals scored significantly lower in BAS- Fun Seeking than those with AN-BP or BN, among which there were no significant difference.
Jiang et al. 74 2010 CPAS 17 subjects with AN-R29 healthy controls Although the AN-R patients had a higher score on the CPAS than healthy controls, the difference failed to reach significance.
Harrison et al. 81 2011 BIS-BAS 29 subjects with AN-R55 subjects with AN-BP34 subjects with BN74 subjects recovered from EDs91 healthy controls The only measure on which the subgroups (AN-R, AN-BP, BN) differed significantly was BAS-FS.AN-R individuals scored significantly lower on the BAS-FS than recovered patients.
Tchanturia et al. 65 2012 RSAS 72 subjects with AN14 subjects with recAN19 subjects with BN43 healthy controls Participants with AN and BN both reported significantly higher levels of social anhedonia than healthy controls, with large effect sizes.
Harrison et al. 66 2014 RSAS 105 subjects with AN30 subjects with recAN46 subjects with BN136 healthy controls Individuals with AN and BN scored significantly higher than healthy controls for the RSAS and the recovered AN group showed an intermediate profile, scoring significantly lower than acute participants and significantly higher than healthy controls.
Tchanturia et al. 68 2014 RSAS 71 subjects with AN: 36 subjects completed CREST A significant decrease in total score on the RSAS between pre and post-intervention was observed.
Tchanturia et al. 69 2015 RSAS 37 subjects with AN: 33 subjects completed CREST A significant decrease in total score on the RSAS between pre and post-intervention was observed.
Lang et al. 67 2016 CSAS 66 subjects with AN75 healthy controls CSAS sores were significantly higher in AN individuals compared to healthy controls.Reduced expression of positive emotions was associated with some clinical variables including elevated social anhedonia.
Boehm et al. 76 2018 SHAPS-D 35 subjects with acute AN who underwent an inpatient treatment program33 subjects with recAN who completed the inpatient treatment programhealthy controls A significantly higher SHAPS-D score was found for acAN and recAN than HC, while no significant difference was found between acAN and recAN. groupsLongitudinally, the SHAPS-D score did not differ significantly before and after weight restoration.
Adamson et al. 70 2018 RSAS 66 subjects with AN who underwent CREST individually62 subjects with AN who underwent a group CREST In both the individual and group format intervention, subjects did not report a significant difference in social anhedonia before and after CREST.
Monteleone A.M. et al. 82 2018 BIS-BAS 48 subjects with AN (38 AN-R;10 AN-BP)30 subjects with BN45 healthy controls Compared to healthy controls, AN and BN individuals reported significant higher scores on sensitivity to punishment (BIS score) but not on reward sensitivity (BAS score).Increased reward sensitivity (BAS-Fun Seeking score) was found in BN individuals compared to AN-R.
Jiang et al. 75 2019 CPAS 14 subjects with AN-R13 subjects with BN12 healthy controls BN patients presented more anhedonia than healthy controls. AN group did not show significant difference from healthy controls.
Harrison et al. 71 2020 RSAS 12 subjects with AN underwent CREST individual format27 subjects with AN underwent CREST group format Significant decrease in social anhedonia at post treatment for both the individual and group format of treatment.
Dolan et al. 77 2021 Anhedonia Subscale of the BDIThe trait subscale of the STAI 499 adults and adolescents in a partial hospitalization program for EDs: 196 AN-R, 69 AN-BP, 103 BN, 15 BED, 26 ARFID, 92 OSFED Anhedonia scores significantly decreased from admission to discharge in all the groupsThe AN-BP group endorsed the highest levels of anhedonia.The ARFID group reported the lowest level of anhedonia and had significantly lower anhedonia than the other diagnostic groups.The OSFED group had significantly higher scores than the AN-R group but did not differ significantly from any other group.
Dolan et al. 78 2022 TEPS 51 subjects with AN40 subjects with BN33 subjects with OSFED83 healthy controls There were no significant difference in the TEPS consummatory scores across individuals with AN, BN, OSFED, and HC.Anticipatory pleasure was found to be significantly higher in healthy controls than in patients with AN and OSFED. There were no significant difference among patients with AN, BN and OSFED.
AN = Anorexia Nervosa; AN-BP = Anorexia Nervosa- Bulimic Purgative subtype; AN-R = Anorexia Nervosa Restricting subtype; rec-AN = Anorexia Nervosa recovered ARFID = Avoidant Restrictive Food Intake Disorder; BED = Binge Eating Disorder; BN = Bulimia Nervosa; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory II; BIS-BAS = Behavioral Inhibition System- Behavioral Activation System; CPAS = Chapman Physical Anhedonia Scale; CREST = Cognitive Remediation and Emotion Skill Training; CSAS = Chapman Social Anhedonia Scale; EDNOS = Eating Disorders not Otherwise Specified; OSFED = Other Specified Eating or Feeding Disorder; RSAS = Revised Chapman Social Anhedonia Scale; SHAPS = Snaith Hamilton Pleasure Scale; STAI = Stait Trait Anxiety Inventory; TEPS = Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scale.
TABLE I. Summary of studies assessing physical and social anhedonia in people with anorexia nervosa.

References

  1. Ribot T. The Psychology of Emotions. W. Scott; 1897.
  2. Loas G. Vulnerability to depression: a model centered on anhedonia. J Affect Disord. 1996;41:39-53. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-0327(96)00065-1
  3. Myerson A. Anhedonia. Am J Psychiatry. 1923;2:87-103.
  4. Dichter G. Anhedonia in Unipolar Major Depressive Disorder: A Review. Open Psychiatry J. 2010;4:1-9. doi:https://doi.org/10.2174/187435440100401000
  5. Pelizza L, Ferrari A. Anhedonia in schizophrenia and major depression: state or trait?. Ann. Gen. Psychiatry. 2009;8:8-22. doi:https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-859X-8-22
  6. Abramovitch A, Pizzagalli D, Reuman L, Wilhelm S. Anhedonia in obsessive-compulsive disorder: beyond comorbid depression. Psychiatry Res. 2014;216:223-229. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.02.002
  7. Loas G, Krystkowiak P, Godefroy O. Anhedonia in Parkinson’s disease: an overview. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2012;24:444-451. doi:https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.neuropsych.11110332
  8. Meehl P. Hedonic capacity: some conjectures. Bull Menninger Clin. 1975;39:295-307.
  9. Kalivas P, Volkow N. The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and choice. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1403-1413. doi:https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1403
  10. Leventhal A, Brightman M, Ameringer K. Anhedonia associated with stimulant use and dependence in a population-based sample of American adults. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2010;18:562-569. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021964
  11. Leventhal A. Relations between anhedonia and physical activity. Am J Health Behav. 2012;36:860-872. doi:https://doi.org/10.5993/AJHB.36.6.12
  12. Cuthbert B, Insel T. Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Med. 2013;11. doi:https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-11-126
  13. Khazanov G, Ruscio A. Is low positive emotionality a specific risk factor for depression? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. 2016;142:991-1015. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000059
  14. Thomsen K. Measuring anhedonia: impaired ability to pursue, experience, and learn about reward. Front Psychol. Published online 2015:6-1409. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01409
  15. Winer E, Bryant J, Bartoszek G. Mapping the relationship between anxiety, anhedonia, and depression. J Affect Disord. 2017;221:289-296. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.06.006
  16. Barkus E, Badcock J. A Transdiagnostic Perspective on Social Anhedonia. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00216
  17. Coniglio K, Christensen K, Haynos A. The posited effect of positive affect in anorexia nervosa: Advocating for a forgotten piece of a puzzling disease. Int J Eat Disord. 2019;52:971-976. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23147
  18. Rizvi S, Pizzagalli D, Sproule B, Kennedy S. Assessing anhedonia in depression: Potentials and pitfalls. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;65:21-35. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubioRev2016.03.004
  19. Snaith R, Hamilton M, Morley S. A scale for the assessment of hedonic tone the Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1995;167:99-103. doi:https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.167.1.99
  20. Fawcett J, Clark D, Scheftner W, Gibbons R. Assessing anhedonia in psychiatric patients. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1983;40:79-84.
  21. Chapman L, Chapman J, Raulin M. Scales for physical and social anhedonia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1976;85:374-382.
  22. Eckblad M, Chapman L, Chapman J, Mishlove M. The Revised Social Anhedonia Scale. Published online 1982.
  23. Gard D, Gard M, Kring A, John O. Anticipatory and consummatory components of the experience of pleasure: A scale development study. J Res Pers. 2006;40:1086-1102. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/jrp.2005.11.001
  24. Llerena K, Park S, McCarthy J. The Motivation and Pleasure Scale-Self-Report (MAP-SR): reliability and validity of a self-report measure of negative symptoms. Compr Psychiatry. 2013;54:568-574. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.12.001
  25. Winer E, Veilleux J, Ginger E. Development and validation of the Specific Loss of Interest and Pleasure Scale (SLIPS). J Affect Disord. 2014;152-154:193-201. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.09.010
  26. Gooding D, Pflum M. The assessment of interpersonal pleasure: introduction of the Anticipatory and Consummatory Interpersonal Pleasure Scale (ACIPS) and preliminary findings. Psychiatry Res. 2014;215:237-243. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.10.012
  27. Gooding D, Pflum M. Further validation of the ACIPS as a measure of social hedonic response. Psychiatry Res. 2014;215:771-777. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.11.009
  28. Rizvi S, Quilty L, Sproule B. Development and validation of the Dimensional Anhedonia Rating Scale (DARS) in a community sample and individuals with major depression. Psychiatry Res. 2015;229:109-119. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.07.062
  29. Gard D, Kring A, Gard M. Anhedonia in schizophrenia: distinctions between anticipatory and consummatory pleasure. Schizophr Res. 2007;93:253-260. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.008
  30. Tso I, Grove T, Taylor S. Differential hedonic experience and behavioral activation in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2014;219:470-476. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.06.030
  31. Carver C, White T. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1994;67:319-333. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319
  32. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders.; 2013.
  33. Campbell I, Mill J, Uher R, Schmidt U. Eating disorders, gene environment interactions and epigenetics. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:784-793. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubioRev2010.09.012
  34. Friederich H, Brooks S, Uher R. Neural correlates of body dissatisfaction in anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:2878-2885. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.036
  35. Friederich H, Walther S, Bendszus M. Grey matter abnormalities within cortico-limbic-striatal circuits in acute and weight-restored anorexia nervosa patients. Neuroimage. 2012;59:1106-1113. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.042
  36. Hasan T, Hasan H. Anorexia nervosa: a unified neurological perspective. Int J Med Sci. 2011;8:679-703. doi:https://doi.org/10.7150/ijms.8.679
  37. Kaye W, Frank G, Bailer U, Henry S. Neurobiology of anorexia nervosa: clinical implications of alterations of the function of serotonin and other neuronal systems. Int J Eat Disord. 2005;37:S15-S19. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20109
  38. Kaye W, Fudge J, Paulus M. New insights into symptoms and neurocircuit function of anorexia nervosa. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:573-584. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2682
  39. Kaye W, Wagner A, Fudge J, Paulus M. 2011. Neurocircuity of eating disorders. Curr Top. Behav Neurosci. 2011;6:37-57. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2010_85
  40. Nunn K, Frampton I, Fuglset T. Anorexia nervosa and the insula. Med Hypotheses. 2011;76:353-357. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2010.10.038
  41. Ogiso K, Asakawa A, Amitani H, Inui A. Ghrelin and anorexia nervosa: a psychosomatic perspective. Nutrition. 2011;27:988-993. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nut.2011.05.005
  42. Treasure J, Cardi V, Kan C. Eating in eating disorders. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2012;20:e42-e49. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.1090
  43. Wierenga C, Ely A, Bischoff-Grethe A. Are extremes of consumption in eating disorders related to an altered balance between reward and inhibition?. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fhbeh.2014.00410
  44. Brooks S, O’Daly O, Uher R. Differential neural responses to food images in women with bulimia versus anorexia nervosa. PLoS One. 2011;6. doi:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022259
  45. Epstein J, Wiseman C, Sunday S. Neurocognitive evidence favors top down over bottom up mechanisms in the pathogenesis of body size distortions in anorexia nervosa. Eat Weight Disord. 2001;6:140-147. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03339763
  46. Kaye W, Wierenga C, Bailer U. Nothing tastes as good as skinny feels: the neurobiology of anorexia nervosa. Trends Neurosci. 2013;36:110-120. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2013.01.003
  47. Berridge K, Chao-Yi H, Richard J, DiFeliceantonio A. 2010. The tempted brain eats: pleasure and desire circuits in obesity and eating disorders. Brain Res. 2010;1350:43-64. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.04.003
  48. Dawe S, Gullo M, Loxton N. Reward drive and rash impulsiveness as dimensions of impulsivity: implications for substance misuse. Addict. Behav. 2004;29:1389-1405. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.06.004
  49. Keating C. Theoretical perspective on anorexia nervosa: the conflict of reward. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34:73-79. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubioRev2009.07.004
  50. Zink C, Weinberger D. Cracking the moody brain: the rewards of self starvation. Nat Med. 2010;16:1382-1383. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/nm1210-1382
  51. Harrison A, O’Brien N, Lopez C. Sensitivity to reward and punishment in eating disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2010;177:1-11. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2009.06.010
  52. Södersten P, Nergardh R, Bergh C. Behavioral neuroendocrinology and treatment of anorexia nervosa. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2008;29:445-462. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2008.06.001
  53. Frank G, Bailer U, Henry S. Increased dopamine D2/D3 receptor binding after recovery from anorexia nervosa measured by positron emission tomography and [11C] raclopride. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58:908-912. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.003
  54. Karson C. Spontaneous eye-blink rates and dopaminergic systems. Brain. 1983;106:643-653. doi:https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/106.3.643
  55. Barbato G, Fichele M, Senatore I. Increased dopaminergic activity in restricting-type anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2006;142:253-255. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2005.07.031
  56. Kalivas P, Volkow N. The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and choice. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1403-1413. doi:https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1403
  57. Schmidt U, Treasure J. Anorexia nervosa: valued and visible. A cognitive interpersonal maintenance model and its implications for research and practice. Br J Clin Psychol. 2006;45:343-366. doi:https://doi.org/10.1348/014466505x53902
  58. Steinhausen H. The outcome of anorexia nervosa in the 20th century. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:1284-1293. doi:https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.159.8.1284
  59. Keyes A, Woerwag-Mehta S, Bartholdy S. Physical activity and the drive to exercise in anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 2015;48:46-54. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22354
  60. Berridge K, Robinson T, Aldridge J. Dissecting components of reward: ‘liking’, ‘wanting’, and learning. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2009;9:65-73. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014
  61. Davis C, Woodside D. Sensitivity to the rewarding effects of food and exercise in the eating disorders. Compr Psychiatry. 2002;43:189-194. doi:https://doi.org/10.1053/comp.2002.32356
  62. Kaye W. Neurobiology of anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Physiol Behav. 2008;94:121-135. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.11.037
  63. Keating C, Tilbrook A, Rossell S. Reward processing in anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:567-75. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.036
  64. Eiber R, Berlin I, de Brettes B. Hedonic response to sucrose solutions and the fear of weight gain in patients with eating disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2002;113:173-80. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-1781(02)00232-9
  65. Tchanturia K, Davies H, Harrison A. Altered social hedonic processing in eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord. 2012;45:962-9. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22032
  66. Harrison A, Mountford V, Tchanturia K. Social anhedonia and work and social functioning in the acute and recovered phases of eating disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2014;218:187-94. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.04.007
  67. Lang K, Larsson E, Mavromara L. Diminished facial emotion expression and associated clinical characteristics in Anorexia Nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2016;236:165-172. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.12.004
  68. Tchanturia K, Doris E, Fleming C. Effectiveness of cognitive remediation and emotion skills training (CREST) for anorexia nervosa in group format: a naturalistic pilot study. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2014;22:200-205. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2287
  69. Tchanturia K, Doris E, Mountford V, Fleming C. Cognitive Remediation and Emotion Skills Training (CREST) for anorexia nervosa in individual format: self-reported outcomes. BMC Psychiatry. 2015;15. doi:https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-015-0434-9
  70. Adamson J, Leppanen J, Murin M, Tchanturia K. Effectiveness of emotional skills training for patients with anorexia nervosa with autistic symptoms in group and individual format. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2018;26:367-375. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2594
  71. Harrison A, Stavri P, Tchanturia K. Individual and group format adjunct therapy on social emotional skills for adolescent inpatients with severe and complex eating disorders (CREST-A). Neuropsychiatr. 2021;35:163-176. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s40211-020-00375-5
  72. Davis C, Scott-Robertson L. A psychological comparison of females with anorexia nervosa and competitive male bodybuilders: body shape ideals in the extreme. Eat Behav. 2000;1:33-46. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/s1471-0153(00)00007-6
  73. Berlin I, Givry-Steiner L, Lecrubier Y, Puech A. Measures of anhedonia and hedonic responses to sucrose in depressive and schizophrenic patients in comparison with healthy subjects. Eur Psychiatry. 1998;13:303-9. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0924-9338(98)80048-5
  74. Jiang T, Soussignan R, Rigaud D, Schaal B. Pleasure for visual and olfactory stimuli evoking energy-dense foods is decreased in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2010;180:42-7. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.04.041
  75. Jiang T, Soussignan R, Carrier E, Royet J. Dysfunction of the Mesolimbic Circuit to Food Odors in Women With Anorexia and Bulimia Nervosa: A fMRI Study. Front Hum Neurosci. 2019;13. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00117
  76. Boehm I, Flohr L, Steding J. The Trajectory of Anhedonic and Depressive Symptoms in Anorexia Nervosa: A Longitudinal and Cross-Sectional Approach. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2018;26:69-74. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2565
  77. Dolan S, Brown T, Wierenga C. Changes in anhedonia over the course of eating disorder treatment. Int J Eat Disord. 2022;55:399-405. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23673
  78. Dolan S, Reilly E, Brown T. Anticipatory and consummatory pleasure in eating disorders. J Eat Disord. 2022;10. doi:https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-022-00692-w
  79. Claes L, Nederkoorn C, Vandereycken W. Impulsiveness and lack of inhibitory control in eating disorders. Eat Behav. 2006;7:196-203. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.05.001
  80. Beck I, Smitsa D, Claes L. Psychometric evaluation of the behavioral inhibition/behavioral activation system scales and the sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward questionnaire in a sample of eating disordered patients. Personal Individ Differ. 2009;47:407-412. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.04.007
  81. Harrison A, Treasure J, Smillie L. 2011. Approach and avoidance motivation in eating disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2011;188:396-401. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.04.022
  82. Monteleone A, Cardi V, Volpe U. Attachment and motivational systems: Relevance of sensitivity to punishment for eating disorder psychopathology. Psychiatry Res. 2018;260:353-359. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.12.002

Downloads

Authors

Rossella Ceres - Department of Medicine, Surgery and Dentistry “Scuola Medica Salernitana”, University of Salerno, Italy

Gianmarco Cascino - Department of Medicine, Surgery and Dentistry “Scuola Medica Salernitana”, University of Salerno, Italy

How to Cite
[1]
Ceres, R. and Cascino, G. 2024. Anhedonia in anorexia nervosa: a narrative review. Journal of Psychopathology. 30, 1 (Feb. 2024). DOI:https://doi.org/10.36148/2284-0249-N458.
  • Abstract viewed - 898 times
  • PDF downloaded - 238 times