This website uses only technical or equivalent cookies.
For more information click here.

Abstract

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder that affects social communication and behavioral routines. Diagnosis is complicated due to the age of symptom onset and the diverse symptomatology. ASD frequently occurs with comorbid psychiatric disorders, including psychotic disorders, that affect the individual’s quality of life, health, and prognosis. The presence of psychosis in ASD is still a debatable topic. We conducted a narrative review to investigate the psychopathological factors that may contribute to the onset of delusion in individuals with ASD, focusing on Attributional style and Theory of Mind (ToM). Although an external Attributional style for negative events is a risk factor for the pathogenesis of delusion in individuals with psychotic spectrum disorders, it seems not to play a pivotal role in the onset of delusions in ASD patients. On the other hand, there is stronger evidence for the lack of ToM in delusional genesis in both psychotic and ASD subjects. To date, the available literature on ToM is still contradictory, and more research is needed, including consideration of social-cognitive deficits and a deeper understanding of the timing of ToM deficit onset in these conditions. Future studies should also investigate the prevalence and type of delusions in ASD patients and their correlation with the severity of autistic symptoms. It is important for modern psychopathology to address these aspects systematically due to the increasing number of ASD diagnoses.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder that affects social communication and behavioral routines. In 1943, Leo Kanner, an Austrian psychiatrist, laid the groundwork for the first definition of childhood autism, describing 11 children, 8 boys and 3 girls, which he presented with “Innate Autistic Disorders of Affective Contact”. In 1980 Autism was first included in DSM-III, which defined it as a lack of responsiveness to other people (autism), a gross impairment in communicative skills, and bizarre responses to various aspects of the environment, all developing within the first 30 months of age. At the time of this publication, the latest version of DSM-5 defined two main criteria for the diagnosis of ASD: the presence of “persistent deficits of social communication and social interaction” and “restricted and repetitive patterns of behavior, interests or activities”. In addition, it is necessary for the symptoms to manifest in early childhood. Ultimately, emphasis is placed on the effect that these symptoms can have on an individual’s daily social and interpersonal interactions 1.

ASD poses a significant challenge in diagnosis, primarily due to the age of symptom onset and its diverse symptomatology. The heterogeneity is marked by varying degrees of severity and considerable fluctuations in symptoms. Additionally, the utilization of a wide range of evaluation methods in the diagnostic procedure further complicates the diagnosis 2. The prevalence of ASD among children in Western societies is estimated to be around 100/10000 but with significant regional differences 3. The incidence is likely to increase as a result of improved detection through screening and diagnostic tests and advancements in clinical evaluation techniques 4,5. In a regional longitudinal record-linkage study conducted in Sweden, including all children up to age 17, they found that the prevalence of ASD increased from 4.2% in 2001 to 14.4% in 2011 6. In Poland, a study conducted between 2010 and 2014 estimated the prevalence of ASD in children up to age 17 to be 5.3% 7. In the province of Pisa, Italy, a prevalence of 0.86% was found in children aged 7 to 9 years 8. Another Italian study found that the cumulative prevalence of autism remained stable from 2001 to 2015, but more cases were diagnosed at an earlier age. The authors emphasized the importance of using cumulative prevalence by time period for a better understanding of ASD occurrence 9.

ASD is frequently found in comorbidity with several clinical and psychiatric disorders 10. Some symptoms are present from birth or evolve during the first years of life, while others may not become apparent until later. In the latter case, socio-economic, cultural, and family contexts may play an important role in the development of ASD. Several reviews have investigated the comorbidity between ASD and various psychiatric and nonpsychiatric disorders. Among the most common health problems are intellectual disability, structural brain abnormalities with or without epilepsy, structural brain changes or abnormalities, and dysmorphic characteristics 9,11,12.

Behavioral and psychiatric comorbidities play a crucial role in the quality of life, health, and prognosis 13. They may include hyperactivity, impulsiveness, aggression, mood disorders, self-harm, and psychotic disorder 14.

Detecting comorbid psychosis in patients with ASD poses a multifaceted dilemma, particularly during the initial stages of psychotic illness and among individuals with ultra-high risk for psychosis (UHR-P), including those with Attenuated Psychosis Syndrome (APS) 15-18.

The presence of psychosis in ASD individuals is still an argument of debate. Kiyono and colleagues performed a meta-analysis published in 2020 showing in ASD a pooled prevalence of psychotic experiences up to 37%, while in the general population a prevalence of psychotic episodes has been reported between 5% to 12%. However, in their subgroup analysis, only 6% of the pooled patients experienced hallucinations, the same as the general population, while 45% experienced delusions 9-21.

Our goal is to investigate the psychopathological factors that may contribute to the onset of delusion in individuals with ASD, with a particular focus on Attributional style and Theory of Mind (ToM). Since our investigation is centered on the mechanisms that involve language, our examination was restricted to individuals diagnosed with level 1 ASD, encompassing those previously diagnosed with Asperger’s Syndrome (AS) or high-functioning autism, according to the diagnostic criteria in effect before the publication of the DSM-5.

Attributional style

Attributional style is a personal and typical way of finding and explaining the causes of events. Abnormalities in attributing meanings can exist in many conditions, such as psychosis and ASD.

Craig and colleagues evaluated ToM and Attributional style in schizophrenia patients with persecutory delusions and compared them to subjects with AS and a healthy control group 22. They used the “Attributional Style Structured Interview”, which asks the participants to describe how they would explain 20 events in four categories: negative events involving a human agent, positive events involving a human agent, negative events not involving a human agent, and positive events not involving a human agent. The “Leeds Attributional Coding System” was utilized to register, transcribe, and codify the answers on five binary dimensions: internal or external, stable or unstable, global or specific, personal or universal, and controllable or uncontrollable. The authors found that subjects with paranoid delusions tended to make external-personal attributions for negative events more than subjects with AS and controls. Both paranoid and AS groups scored worse on the ToM tasks than the control group. Also, the paranoia-related scores were higher compared to the control group. This suggests that ToM and the attributional abnormalities contribute to paranoid delusions. However, the lack of attributional abnormalities in the AS group implies that paranoid delusion results from different mechanisms than those involved in the delusions of schizophrenic patients 22.

Another study by Aakre and colleagues compared patients who suffer from paranoid delusion with those without paranoid delusion and a healthy control group. They similarly revealed a greater tendency to make external-personal attributions for negative events in patients with paranoid delusion 23.

Didehbani and colleagues conducted a study in 2012 examining insight into illness and social attributional styles in individuals with AS compared with healthy controls. There was an inverse relationship between externalizing bias and level of insight in individuals with AS, i.e., a high level of insight was associated with a tendency for self-blame. This association was particularly evident when assessed with a clinician-administered insight measure compared to a self-reported measure and may be related to an increased awareness when externally assessed 24.

Blackshaw and colleagues investigated Attributional styles in a cohort of individuals with AS. The authors found that subjects with AS scored lower points on ToM tests and higher points on paranoia tests compared to the control group. However, the two groups had no significant differences regarding the dimension of meaning attribution. The paranoia observed in individuals with AS is therefore believed to stem from different factors compared to those that contribute to paranoia in individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia. The latter may arise as a strategy used as a defense from a subject-threatening plot, while the former may stem from confusion about not understanding the subtleties of social interactions and rules. This would explain why abnormal meaning attributions were not found in subjects with AS 25.

ToM in ASD and delusions

ToM explains the ability to infer other individuals’ mental states and is a fundamental element of human social interactions 26,27. Some mental states lead to deficiencies in the ability to perceive any perspective other than their own. ToM-deficits have been described in various neurodevelopmental disorders, such as ASD, attention-deficit hyperactive disorder (ADHD), and schizophrenia, as well as in acquired neuropsychiatric disorders, such as various brain injuries and dementia 28,29. Current models have a differentiated approach to ToM, separating cognitive ToM, affective ToM, and a spectrum of a continuum from hypo- to hyper-ToM 30-35. Cognitive ToM explores the cognitive ability to recognize the mental state of others, including their thoughts and intentions, while affective ToM is an empathic evaluation of another individual’s emotional state 34. Hyper-ToM is described as an excessive presence of ToM that leads to an over-attribution of knowledge to others, resulting in inaccurate conclusions about their mental state 35.

In individuals with ASD, the primary features include difficulties in shared social interaction with verbal and non-verbal communication deficits and stereotyped repetitive behavior. To a various degree, there is an inability to assess the behavior of other individuals based on their mental states. Thus in ASD, a deficit in ToM has been linked to the core autistic symptomatology 36,37. An early study by Baron-Cohen and colleagues performed on children with and without ASD showed a lack of ToM in children with ASD 37. Further studies on adults and children on more demanding ToM tasks showed an association with inferior social cognitive performance 23,38-41. Also, there are differences along the ASD spectrum as there is high individual variance on tasks designed to assess the ToM, and individuals performing better on ToM tests are better socially integrated 23,42,43. Not all aspects of ASD can be credited to a deficit in ToM 44,45, but its effect on social skills is significant 46.

The notion of a dysfunctional ToM in individuals with ASD is not without controversies. As discussed in a recent review, deficits in ToM are due to impaired communication skills – a criterion for the DSM-5-TR diagnosis of ASD – and the authors argue that the evidence for dysfunction in ToM is based on a poor methodological foundation 47.

The first mention of a relationship between ToM and delusion came from Frith in 1992. Frith postulated that a lack of ToM was a predisposing cognitive factor for delusion and stated that paranoid delusions occur because the patient makes incorrect assumptions about other people’s intentions 48. Since then, ToM has been extensively studied in patients with psychotic disorders establishing an association with dysfunction in ToM 49-52. In several studies, altered ToM abilities were linked not only with the diagnosis of schizophrenia but also with specific psychotic delusional symptoms. More specifically, some studies investigated impaired ToM in patients with persecutory delusions showing that those patients presented pronounced debts in various ToM capacities, including the ability to infer emotions and intentions and to understand second-order false beliefs 31,53-56.

Regarding hyper-ToM, most studies support an association between hyper-ToM errors with positive symptoms and delusion 32,53,56,57. However, Dorn and colleagues recently found no association between hyper-ToM errors and psychotic symptoms, whether positive, negative, or disorganized 58.

Establishing a connection between ToM, delusion, and ASD is challenging due to the lack of literature on the topic. The notion of delusional beliefs in patients with ASD has initially been limited to case reports 59-62. However, in the last two decades, experimental studies have established a meaningful relationship, which was discussed in our recent review 17. Furthermore, individuals with ASD regularly present dysfunctions in ToM 63. Assuming that dysfunction in ToM is a predictor of delusion in patients with psychotic disorder, one could presume its importance in delusion also in individuals with ASD.

As previously mentioned, Blackshaw with associates showed higher scores on measures of paranoia and lower scores on ToM tasks in subjects diagnosed with AS 25; on the contrary, Abell and Hare found no association between ToM ability and general delusion or paranoia in individuals with AS 64. Instead, they propose a model of development and maintenance of delusional beliefs in people with AS in which ToM indirectly influences individual life experiences and renders them vulnerable to developing delusional beliefs.

When do ToM impairments and biased attributional styles appear in psychosis?

Several studies examined ToM deficits in psychosis 51,65. However, the timing of ToM impairment onset in psychosis is not completely clear. While ASD children never seem to acquire ToM abilities 48, schizophrenic patients and brain-injured patients start showing severe ToM impairments only after their disease outbreak. Understanding when ToM deficits occur in schizophrenia might provide important information on schizophrenia pathogenesis and delusional formation. Bora and colleagues conducted a meta-analysis evaluating impairments in first-episode psychosis (FEP), ultra-high risk (UHR) subjects, and first-degree healthy relatives of schizophrenic patients 66. The analysis showed that ToM was significantly impaired in UHR subjects and unaffected relatives compared to healthy controls. On the other hand, the severity of ToM impairment in FEP was close to the chronic patients’ score reported in a precedent meta-analysis 49. These findings seem to suggest that ToM impairments gradually appear during the early stages of schizophrenia and definitely erupt with large effect sizes after the explosion of the psychotic onset. Consequently, ToM-deficits do not seem to be explained by the effects of illness progression, chronicity, and long-term pharmacotherapy. These results are still not conclusive, and more recent research was not able to replicate them 67.

Concerning Attribution styles, Parks and colleagues investigated whether UHR individuals show increased hostility perception and blaming bias and explored the associations of these biased styles of attribution with the factor structure of multifaceted self-related psychological variables and neurocognitive performances. Similar to full-blown psychotic patients, UHR individuals showed increased hostility perception and blaming bias when compared with normal controls, supporting the emergence of attribution biases even in the putative prodromal phase of schizophrenia 68.

Ultimately, while emerging results are in favor of ToM-defictis playing a role in the pathogenesis of delusions in both ASD and schizophrenia, biased Attributional style seems to play a major role in paranoid delusions in psychotic patients. The dual role of ToM deficits in delusion formation could be attributed to the tight overlap and similarity between AS and schizotypy, as a significant association between positive and negative schizotypy and lower ToM performance has been demonstrated 49. Additionally, since different aspects of the self have been reported to serve as accounting factors for attributional styles 68, further studies should evaluate the correlation between these dimensions and abnormalities in attributional style in level 1 or high-functioning ASD subjects.

Conclusions

The pathogenesis of delusions in patients with ASD is complex, and no clear psychopathological explanations exist. Unfortunately, few data are available in the literature.

According to our review, Attributional style does not appear to be related to the onset of delusions in ASD patients compared to psychotic patients.

Regarding ToM, there is stronger evidence for the lack of ToM in ASD individuals. However, the data are contradictory, and no definitive conclusions can be drawn. To better assess the relationship between ToM and delusion, it would be important to consider social-cognitive deficits such as cognitive and affective ToM, as well as measure the spectrum of a continuum from hypo- to hyper-ToM.

There are several considerations to be made. First, recognizing psychotic symptoms in ASD patients is complex and requires in-depth psychopathological knowledge. It is, therefore, possible that delusional symptoms are frequently not recognized. Furthermore, non-persecutory delusional symptoms may be more present in ASD patients, such as “expansive delusions” 59 or “unusual thoughts” 61. These non-persecutory delusions are generally not related to any bias in meaning attributions, which could explain the lack of data about the role of Attribution style in the onset of delusions in ASD patients. Likewise, ToM deficits are more commonly associated with persecutory delusions.

In this regard, it would be helpful to conduct future studies to better characterize the prevalence and type of delusions in ASD patients. Furthermore, it would be necessary to investigate the correlation between the severity of autistic symptoms, particularly deficits in social communication, and the prevalence of delusional symptoms. In light of the exponential increase in ASD diagnoses, it is mandatory for modern psychopathology to address these aspects systematically.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agencies in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Authors’ contributions

MR and GDL: study conceptualization and design; EE, FFN, CF, SF, GA: literature search; MR: wrote the first draft of the manuscript; AS, CN, MR and GDL: supervision of the manuscript; all the Authors reviewed and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Ethical consideration

Not applicable.

References

  1. Rosen N, Lord C, Volkmar F. The Diagnosis of Autism: from Kanner to DSM-III to DSM-5 and Beyond. J Autism Dev Disord. 2021;51:4253-4270. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-04904-1
  2. Bougeard C, Picarel-Blanchot F, Schmid R. Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorder and co-morbidities in children and adolescents: a systematic literature review. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.744709
  3. Li Y-A, Chen Z-J, Li X-D. Epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders: Global burden of disease 2019 and bibliometric analysis of risk factors. Front Pediatr. Published online 2022. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fped.2022.972809
  4. Zeidan J, Fombonne E, Scorah J. Global prevalence of autism: A systematic review update. Autism Res. 2022;15:778-790. doi:https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2696
  5. Chiarotti F, Venerosi A. Epidemiology of Autism Spectrum Disorders: a review of worldwide prevalence estimates since 2014. Brain Sci. 2020;10. doi:https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10050274
  6. Idring S, Lundberg M, Sturm H. Changes in prevalence of autism spectrum disorders in 2001-2011: findings from the Stockholm youth cohort. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:1766-1773. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2336-y
  7. Skonieczna-Żydecka K, Gorzkowska I, Pierzak-Sominka J. The Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorders in West Pomeranian and Pomeranian Regions of Poland. J Appl Res Intellect Disabil. 2017;30:283-289. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/jar.12238
  8. Narzisi A, Posada M, Barbieri F. Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorder in a large Italian catchment area: a school-based population study within the ASDEU project. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2018;29. doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796018000483
  9. Valenti M, Vagnetti R, Masedu F. Register-based cumulative prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorders during childhood and adolescence in Central Italy. Epidemiol Biostat Public Health. Published online 2019.
  10. Mazzone L, Ruta L, Reale L. Psychiatric comorbidities in asperger syndrome and high functioning autism: diagnostic challenges. Ann Gen Psychiatry. 2012;11. doi:https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-859X-11-16
  11. Rapin I. Autistic regression and disintegrative disorder: how important the role of epilepsy. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 1995;2:278-285. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/s1071-9091(95)80007-7
  12. Lord C, Brugha T, Charman T. Autism spectrum disorder. Nature Reviews Disease Primers. 2020;6:1-23. doi:https://doi.org/10.1038/s41572-019-0138-4
  13. Barlattani T, D’Amelio C, Cavatassi A. Autism spectrum disorders and psychiatric comorbidities: a narrative review. Journal of Psychopathology. 2023;29:3-24. doi:https://doi.org/10.36148/2284-0249-N281
  14. Genovese A, Butler M. Clinical assessment, genetics, and treatment approaches in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Int J Mol Sci. 2020;21. doi:https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms21134726
  15. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder. American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2022.
  16. Ribolsi M, Albergo G, Fiori Nastro F. Autistic symptomatology in UHR patients: A preliminary report. Psychiatry Res,. 2022;313. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2022.114634
  17. Ribolsi M, Fiori NF, Pelle M. Recognizing psychosis in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front Psychiatry. 2022;13. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.768586
  18. Riccioni A, Siracusano M, Vasta M. Clinical profile and conversion rate to full psychosis in a prospective cohort study of youth affected by autism spectrum disorder and attenuated psychosis syndrome: A preliminary report. Front Psychiatry. 2022;13. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.950888
  19. Kiyono T, Morita M, Morishima R. The prevalence of psychotic experiences in Autism Spectrum Disorder and Autistic Traits: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin Open. Published online 2020.
  20. Linscott R, van Os J. An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychol Med. 2013;43:1133-1149. doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291712001626
  21. McGrath J, Saha S, Al-Hamzawi A. Psychotic experiences in the general population: a cross-national analysis based on 31,261 respondents from 18 countries. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72:697-705. doi:https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0575
  22. Craig J, Hatton C, Craig F. Persecutory beliefs, attributions and theory of mind: comparison of patients with paranoid delusions, Asperger’s syndrome and healthy controls. Schizophr Res. 2004;69:29-33. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00154-3
  23. Aakre J, Seghers J, St-Hilaire A. Attributional style in delusional patients: a comparison of remitted paranoid, remitted nonparanoid, and current paranoid patients with nonpsychiatric controls. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:994-1002. doi:https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbn033
  24. Didehbani N, Shad M, Kandalaft M. Brief report: insight into illness and social attributional style in Asperger’s syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42:2754-2760. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1532-x
  25. Blackshaw A, Kinderman P, Hare D. Theory of mind, causal attribution and paranoia in Asperger syndrome. Autism. 2001;5:147-163. doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361301005002005
  26. Poulin-Dubois D. Theory of mind development: State of the science and future directions. Prog Brain Res. 2020;254:141-166. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2020.05.021
  27. Wimmer H, Perner J. Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition. 1983;13:103-128. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
  28. Korkmaz B. Theory of mind and neurodevelopmental disorders of childhood. Pediatr Res. 2011;69:101R-8R. doi:https://doi.org/10.1203/PDR.0b013e318212c177
  29. Martín-Rodríguez J, León-Carrión J. Theory of mind deficits in patients with acquired brain injury: a quantitative review. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:1181-1191. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.009
  30. Brothers L, Ring B. A neuroethological framework for the representation of minds. J Cogn Neurosci. 1992;4:107-118. doi:https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1992.4.2.107
  31. Mehl S, Rief W, Lüllmann E. Are theory of mind deficits in understanding intentions of others associated with persecutory delusions. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2010;198:516-519. doi:https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181e4c8d2
  32. Montag C, Dziobek I, Richter I. Different aspects of theory of mind in paranoid schizophrenia: evidence from a video-based assessment. Psychiatry Res. 2011;186:203-209. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.09.006
  33. Nelson K, Murphy S, Bucci W. Narrativity and referential activity predict episodic memory strength in autobiographical memories. J Psycholinguist Res. 2021;50:103-16. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-021-09763-6
  34. Shamay-Tsoory S, Shur S, Barcai-Goodman L. Dissociation of cognitive from affective components of theory of mind in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2007;149:11-23. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2005.10.018
  35. Abu-Akel A, Bailey A. The possibility of different forms of theory of mind impairment in psychiatric and developmental disorders. Psychol Med. 2000;30:735-38. doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291799002123
  36. David ZP, Jacques S, Burack J. The relation between theory of mind and rule use: evidence from persons with autism-spectrum disorders. Infant and Child Development. 2002;11:171-95.
  37. Baron-Cohen S, Leslie A, Frith U. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind.” Cognition. 1985;21:37-46. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8
  38. Brent E, Rios P, Happé F. Performance of children with autism spectrum disorder on advanced theory of mind tasks. Autism. 2004;8:283-299. doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361304045217
  39. Heavey L, Phillips W, Baron-Cohen S. The Awkward Moments Test: a naturalistic measure of social understanding in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30:225-236. doi:https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1005544518785
  40. Jolliffe T, Baron-Cohen S. The Strange Stories Test: a replication with high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 1999;29:395-406. doi:https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1023082928366
  41. Kleinman J, Marciano P, Ault R. Advanced theory of mind in high-functioning adults with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31:29-36. doi:https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1005657512379
  42. Livingston L, Colvert E, Social R. Good social skills despite poor theory of mind: exploring compensation in autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2019;60:102-110. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12886
  43. Peterson C, Garnett M, Kelly A. Everyday social and conversation applications of theory-of-mind understanding by children with autism-spectrum disorders or typical development. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;18:105-115. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-008-0711-y
  44. Altschuler M, Sideridis G, Kala S. Measuring individual differences in cognitive, affective, and spontaneous theory of mind among school-aged children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48:3945-3957. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3663-1
  45. Boucher J. Putting theory of mind in its place: psychological explanations of the socio-emotional-communicative impairments in autistic spectrum disorder. Autism. 2012;16:226-46. doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361311430403
  46. Begeer S, Malle B, Nieuwland M. Using Theory of Mind to represent and take part in social interactions: comparing individuals with high-functioning autism and typically developing controls. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2010;7:104-22.
  47. Gernsbacher M, Yergeau M. Empirical failures of the claim that autistic people lack a theory of mind. Arch Sci Psychol. 2019;7:102-118. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/arc0000067
  48. Frith C. The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia (Classic Edition). Psychology press; 2015.
  49. Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2009;109:1-9. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2008.12.020
  50. Corcoran R, Cahill C, Frith C. The appreciation of visual jokes in people with schizophrenia: a study of ‘mentalizing’ ability. Schizophr Res. 1997;24:319-327. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/s0920-9964(96)00117-x
  51. Sprong M, Schothorst P, Vos E. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;191:5-13. doi:https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.107.035899
  52. Tényi T, Herold R, Szili I. Schizophrenics show a failure in the decoding of violations of conversational implicatures. Psychopathology. 2002;35:25-27. doi:https://doi.org/10.1159/000056212
  53. Blakemore S, Sarfati Y, Bazin N. The detection of intentional contingencies in simple animations in patients with delusions of persecution. Psychol Med. 2003;33:1433-41. doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291703008341
  54. Gawęda Ł, Prochwicz K. A comparison of cognitive biases between schizophrenia patients with delusions and healthy individuals with delusion-like experiences. Eur Psychiatry. 2015;30:943-949. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.08.003
  55. Phalen P, Dimaggio G, Popolo R. Aspects of Theory of Mind that attenuate the relationship between persecutory delusions and social functioning in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2017;56:65-70. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.07.008
  56. Fretland R, Andersson S, Sundet K. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: error types and associations with symptoms. Schizophr Res. 2015;162:42-46. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.01.024
  57. Peyroux E, Prost Z, Danset-Alexandre C. From “under” to “over” social cognition in schizophrenia: Is there distinct profiles of impairments according to negative and positive symptoms. Schizophr Res Cogn. 2019;15:21-29. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2018.10.001
  58. Dorn L, Struck N, Bitsch F. The Relationship between different aspects of Theory of Mind and symptom clusters in psychotic disorders: Deconstructing theory of mind into cognitive, affective, and hyper theory of mind. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.607154
  59. Clarke D, Baxter M, Perry D. The diagnosis of affective and psychotic disorders in adults with autism: seven case reports. Autism. 1999;3:149-164.
  60. Clarke D, LittleJohns C, Corbett J. Pervasive developmental disorders and psychoses in adult life. Br J Psychiatry. 1989;155:692-699.
  61. Darr G, Worden F. Case report twenty-eight years after an infantile autistic disorder. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1951;21:559-570. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1951.tb00012.x
  62. Szatmari P. Asperger’s syndrome: diagnosis, treatment, and outcome. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1991;14:81-93.
  63. Tin L, Lui S, Ho K. High-functioning autism patients share similar but more severe impairments in verbal theory of mind than schizophrenia patients. Psychol Med. 2018;48:1264-1273. doi:https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717002690
  64. Abell F, Hare D. An experimental investigation of the phenomenology of delusional beliefs in people with Asperger syndrome. Autism. 2005;9:515-531. doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361305057857
  65. Bora E, Gökçen S, Kayahan B. Deficits of social-cognitive and social-perceptual aspects of theory of mind in remitted patients with schizophrenia: effect of residual symptoms. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2008;196:95-99. doi:https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e318162a9e1
  66. Bora E, Pantelis C. Theory of mind impairments in first-episode psychosis, individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis and in first-degree relatives of schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2013;144:31-36. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.12.013
  67. Kong W, Koo S, Seo E. Empathy and Theory of Mind in ultra-high risk for psychosis: relations with schizotypy and executive function. Psychiatry Investig. 2021;18:1109-1116. doi:https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2021.0111
  68. Park H, Bang M, Kim K. Fragile self and malevolent others: biased attribution styles in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis. Psychiatry Investig. 2018;15:796-804. doi:https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2018.05.08

Downloads

Authors

Michele Ribolsi - Unit of Neurology, Neurophysiology, Neurobiology and Psychiatry, Department of Medicine, University Campus Bio-Medico of Rome, Rome, Italy

Eleonora Esposto - Department of Systems Medicine, Tor Vergata University of Rome, Rome, Italy

Federico Fiori Nastro - Department of Systems Medicine, Tor Vergata University of Rome, Rome, Italy

Chiara Falvo - Department of Systems Medicine, Tor Vergata University of Rome, Rome, Italy

Simone Fieramosca - Department of Systems Medicine, Tor Vergata University of Rome, Rome, Italy

Giuliano Albergo - Unit of Neurology, Neurophysiology, Neurobiology and Psychiatry, Department of Medicine, University Campus Bio-Medico of Rome, Rome, Italy

Cinzia Niolu - Department of Systems Medicine, Tor Vergata University of Rome, Rome, Italy

Alberto Siracusano - Department of Systems Medicine, Tor Vergata University of Rome, Rome, Italy

Giorgio Di Lorenzo - 2 Department of Systems Medicine, Tor Vergata University of Rome, Rome, Italy; 3 IRCCS-Fondazione Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy

How to Cite
[1]
Ribolsi, M., Esposto, E., Fiori Nastro, F. , Falvo, C., Fieramosca, S., Albergo, G., Niolu, C., Siracusano, A. and Di Lorenzo, G. 2023. The onset of delusion in autism spectrum disorder: a psychopathological investigation. Journal of Psychopathology. 29, 1/2 (Jul. 2023). DOI:https://doi.org/10.36148/2284-0249-N282.
  • Abstract viewed - 3739 times
  • PDF downloaded - 316 times